Sign In
thar tribune thar tribune
  • Politics & Government
  • Music & Entertainment
  • Law & Crime
  • LGBTQ+ & Women’s Rights
  • Offbeat
  • Science & Technology
  • More
    • About Us
    • Contact Us
    • Privacy Policy
    • Terms of Use
    • Disclaimer
    • Bookmarks
Reading: When Monkeys Learned the Value of Money and One Quickly Traded It for Sex
Share
Thar TribuneThar Tribune
Font ResizerAa
Search
  • Home
    • About Us
    • Contact Us
    • Privacy Policy
    • Terms of Use
    • Disclaimer
  • Categories
    • Politics & Government
    • Music & Entertainment
    • Law & Crime
    • LGBTQ+ & Women’s Rights
    • Offbeat
    • Science & Technology
  • Bookmarks
Have an existing account? Sign In
Follow US
© Vari Media Pvt Ltd 2023 – 2024. All rights reserved. See terms of use. Thar Tribune is not responsible for the content of any third-party websites.
Offbeat

When Monkeys Learned the Value of Money and One Quickly Traded It for Sex

Prathamesh Kabra
Last updated: July 15, 2025 6:26 AM
By Prathamesh Kabra
Share
11 Min Read
SHARE

Most of us like to believe that money is a human invention tied to our unique intelligence and culture. Coins and bills seem so bound to cities, markets, and long histories of trade that we imagine no other animal could ever touch those concepts.

Yet in 2005, two researchers at Yale University decided to test that belief with seven capuchin monkeys. Economist Keith Chen and psychologist Laurie Santos were curious, even mischievous, in their thinking. If monkeys could learn symbols, what would happen if those symbols carried value?

They began with simple silver discs punched through the center, smooth tokens that meant nothing to a capuchin. Training took weeks of repetition. The monkeys were shown that one disc could be exchanged for a piece of apple, or grape, or a cube of Jell‑O.

At first the animals hesitated, sniffing and pawing at the discs. But slowly, understanding appeared. A monkey would grasp a token, hand it to a researcher, and eagerly grab its reward. The tokens were no longer shiny toys. They were suddenly worth something.

Chen loved to describe capuchins in blunt, colorful ways. He called them “bottomless stomachs of want.” Give them marshmallows until they vomit and they will still reach for more. Food and sex are their engines, he said, and you could almost see their hunger shaping every choice.

When the experiment matured, each monkey was given a small supply of tokens before entering a chamber filled with food stands. Grapes cost more than apple slices. Jell‑O was cheaper. The animals moved between options, spending their tokens with choices that mirrored human shoppers.

Researchers changed prices to see if the capuchins adapted. They did. When Jell‑O dropped in cost, monkeys shifted toward it, just as human consumers favor cheaper goods. The animals budgeted, calculated, and displayed basic economic reasoning that startled even the experts.

The team pushed further. They introduced gambling games with uncertain rewards. The monkeys played eagerly, falling into the same irrational patterns human gamblers know too well. Statistically, Chen remarked, their behavior was indistinguishable from real investors on a stock floor.

This was not Chen’s first dive into primate economics. Years earlier at Harvard, he worked with Marc Hauser on altruism studies in cotton‑top tamarins. Two monkeys were placed in separate cages, each controlling a lever that delivered food to the other.

Many tamarins pulled the lever about forty percent of the time, suggesting some willingness to benefit another without reward. Then researchers added twists. One monkey was trained to always pull the lever, regardless of response. Another was trained never to pull it at all.

The mindless altruist kept giving food without hesitation. Other monkeys quickly noticed and reduced their own efforts to about thirty percent, exploiting the automatic giver. The selfish one faced fury. Chen remembers monkeys flinging feces, sulking in corners, and clearly resenting the refusal to play fair.

These side studies showed how quickly primates sense patterns and change behavior. So when Chen and Santos brought the token economy to capuchins, they knew they might see unexpected social ripples. They just did not predict how human those ripples would look.

Once the basic trading worked, researchers handed out twelve tokens per monkey and watched carefully. The chamber became a small market, and each animal carried its coins like a shopper wandering through stalls. It was both hilarious and eerie to witness.

The capuchins showed no urge to save. Every token burned a hole in their tiny palms. Some tried to steal, slipping an extra disc from the tray when the humans were distracted. Chaos erupted once when a bold monkey grabbed the entire tray and fled.

Screeches filled the lab as others chased him. Tokens clattered across the floor. It was as if a bank robbery had just taken place in miniature, with furious crowds scrambling after the loot. Researchers scrambled too, stunned by the sudden eruption of greed.

The chaos revealed something deeper. In the confusion, one male monkey approached a female, exchanged a token, and immediately engaged in sexual behavior. Witnesses froze. When the brief encounter ended, the female marched straight to the food stand and bought a grape.

This moment, unexpected and almost absurd, echoed human history in uncomfortable ways. Within weeks of teaching them currency, the researchers had seen not only budgeting, gambling, and theft but also the world’s first recorded instance of monkey prostitution.

Chen spoke about it later with a mix of amazement and unease. The monkeys had taken a human invention and wrapped it into their primal needs. Food and sex had always been their motivators, and money simply became another tool to pursue them.

The researchers even tried trick experiments. They cut cucumbers into round slices mimicking tokens and handed them to the monkeys as if they were food. One monkey chewed thoughtfully, then carried the cucumber slice to a researcher, hoping to trade it for something better.

These details matter because they prove the monkeys were not just conditioned. They were forming abstract links. A token, even if edible, might hold value beyond itself. It could be exchanged. It could bring something richer, something tastier, something worth the effort.

Chen and Santos documented every move, every shift in market behavior, and every spark of chaos. They watched as theft repeated. They watched as cooperation faltered. They watched as token economies shaped a small primate society, complete with its own dark corners.

The project began almost as a curiosity, a thought experiment turned real. But the data it produced fed into larger questions about human nature. If monkeys mirror our economic choices, how much of our own behavior is ancient, wired deep into mammal brains?

It is easy to laugh at the idea of a capuchin counting coins, but the research forces us to reflect. We like to think money is a cultural agreement. Yet here were animals grasping its power after only months of training.

The Yale team kept refining methods. They observed how scarcity changed demand. They tracked how tokens circulated and how quickly markets adapted. They saw tokens stolen, hoarded, and spent in ways no different from crowded human marketplaces.

In interviews, Chen marveled at their capacity to budget and gamble yet expressed caution. He warned that when you give a capuchin access to currency, you unleash every raw instinct. Their society becomes a mirror of ours, with vices exposed almost immediately.

The experiment also opened doors for new studies on decision making. Economists saw value in comparing primal trade behavior with modern financial systems. Psychologists saw glimpses of evolutionary roots behind greed, cooperation, and exploitation.

It is tempting to frame the story as comedy. Tiny monkeys clutching tokens, paying for grapes, fleeing with loot. Yet beneath the humor lies a serious insight into how easily abstract symbols can govern behavior across species.

The research paper, titled “How Basic Are Behavioral Biases? Evidence From Capuchin Monkey Trading Behavior,” still circulates among scholars. It stands as proof that money is not only about culture but also about deep patterns in the mind.

When you picture those seven capuchins, think of them not as simple animals but as early participants in a market older than we admit. Their paws gripped silver discs and opened a window into our own evolution.

The image of a female monkey taking a token after mating and walking calmly to buy a grape is almost poetic in its strangeness. In that single act, the line between human and animal behavior blurred in a way few expected.

Chen once joked that if you fed them enough, they would vomit and return for more. Watching them gamble, steal, and trade sex for food, you begin to wonder how far removed we are from that same engine of want.

Laurie Santos, reflecting on the chaos, admitted that the experiment went far beyond its original scope. They had set out to see if a monkey could buy a grape. They ended up creating a miniature economy with all the flaws of our own.

And so the story remains, bizarre and unforgettable. In a Yale lab, seven capuchins learned the power of a token. They learned that silver discs could open doors to desire. And before long, those same discs carried them into behaviors that sounded painfully familiar.

It leaves us with a curious thought. Perhaps the roots of commerce, temptation, and even moral compromise run far deeper than we care to admit. Watching those capuchins, one can almost hear history whisper that our own markets began the very same way.

Subscribe to Our Newsletter

Subscribe to our newsletter to get our newest articles instantly!

Share This Article
Twitter Email Copy Link Print
Previous Article An investigation into the baffling disappearance of Leonard Dirickson, who left his Oklahoma ranch one morning in 1998 and vanished without a trace
Next Article Scotland’s Longest Search: What Happened to Sandy Davidson?
Leave a comment

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

[adinserter name="Sidebar"]

Related Articles

Law & CrimeOffbeat

Scotland’s Longest Search: What Happened to Sandy Davidson?

11 Min Read
OffbeatLaw & Crime

An investigation into the baffling disappearance of Leonard Dirickson, who left his Oklahoma ranch one morning in 1998 and vanished without a trace

13 Min Read
OffbeatLaw & Crime

The Forgotten Children of Fernald: How MIT’s Science Club Hid a Radioactive Secret

11 Min Read
Law & CrimeOffbeat

Rey Rivera Took a Call and Vanished. What Happened After Still Makes No Sense.

33 Min Read
thar tribune thar tribune

Thar Tribune Site

  • About Us
  • Contact Us
  • Privacy Policy
  • Terms of Use
  • Disclaimer

Selected Topics

  • Politics & Government
  • Music & Entertainment
  • Law & Crime
  • LGBTQ+ & Women’s Rights
  • Offbeat
  • Science & Technology

Selected Writers

  • Kriti Shrivastava
  • Prathamesh Kabra

Vari Media Pvt Ltd

Nathalal Parekh Marg, Matunga, Mumbai – 400019, 
Maharashtra, India

© Vari Media Pvt Ltd 2023 – 2024. All rights reserved. See terms of use. Thar Tribune is not responsible for the content of any third-party websites.

Welcome Back!

Sign in to your account

Lost your password?